The effects of lexical densities and task types on Chinese students’ incidental vocabulary acquisition

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.54475/jlt.2024.014

Keywords:

incidental vocabulary acquisition, lexical densities, task types, Chinese senior high students

Abstract

This study examines the effects arose by different lexical densities (2%, 5%) and task types (mind map, continuation task) on Chinese senior high students. Four classes of intermediate-level Chinese EFL learners were randomly assigned to four groups, where the treatment condition for each group includes two variables (group1: 2%+mind map, group2: 5%+mind map, group3: 5%+continuation task, group4: 2%+continuation task). It was found that (1) lexical density (2%, 5%) would not result in a significant difference on incidental vocabulary acquisition; (2) Both tasks would lead to a significant difference on incidental vocabulary acquisition and (3) mind map showed superiority in enhancing receptive vocabulary, productive vocabulary and word retention under the text of 5% of lexical density; (4) 2% of lexical density more facilitated output vocabulary when the continuation task was administered. Pedagogical implications of these results for EFL vocabulary acquisition are discussed.

Author Biography

  • Zibei Ye, Anshun University

    Zibei Ye is an Assistant Teacher at Anshun University in Guizhou, China. She is responsible for teaching public English to sophomores. Her research fields include second language acquisition and language teaching. She graduated with a master’s degree from Hunan University. She has participated in projects such as cognitive linguistics and foreign language teaching, as well as computer assisted language teaching.
    Email: 593024755@qq.com or yezibefh@163.com

References

Adolphs, S., & Schmitt, N. (2004). Vocabulary coverage according to spoken discourse context. In P. Bogaards & B. Laufer (Eds.), Vocabulary in a second language (pp. 39-49). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.10.05ado DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.10.05ado

Atkinson, D., Churchill, E., Nishino, T., & Okada, H. (2007). Alignment and interaction in a sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition. The Modern Language Journal, 91(2),169-188. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00539.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00539.x

Alqahtani, M. (2015). The importance of vocabulary in language learning and how to be taught. International Journal of Teaching and Education, 3(3), 21-34. https://doi.org/10.20472/TE.2015.3.3.002 DOI: https://doi.org/10.20472/TE.2015.3.3.002

Burkhard, R. A., & Eppler, J. (2004). Knowledge visualization towards a new discipline and its fields of application. USI Res. Note, 2.

Cui, Y., Yang, L., & Wolter, B. (2022). Alignment effect in the continuation task of Chinese low-intermediate English learners. Applied Linguistics Review, 13(4), 501-526. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2019-0035

Dai, C. (2022). Exploring the relationship of cohesion and coherence in test-takers’ written texts with their writing scores in a continuation task (Master's thesis, Queen's University (Canada)).

Gass, S. (1999). Discussion: Incidental vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21(2), 319-333. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263199002090 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263199002090

Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 8-11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.016 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.016

Hue, H. C., & Nation, P. (2000). Unknown word density and reading comprehension. Reading in Foreign Language, 13(1), 403-430.

Hulstijn, J. H., & Laufer, B. (2001). Some empirical evidence for the involvement load hypothesis in vocabulary acquisition. Language Learning, 51(3), 539-558. https://doi.org/10.1111/0023-8333.00164 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/0023-8333.00164

Kıvrak, C. (2019). The effects of task involvement load and input type on foreign language vocabulary learning (Master’s thesis, Marmara Universitesi (Turkey)). DOI: https://doi.org/10.17263/jlls.668478

Laufer, B., & Ravenhorst-Kalovski, G. C. (2010). Lexical threshold revisited: Lexical text coverage, learners’ vocabulary size and reading comprehension.

Laufer, B. (2016). The three “I” s of second language vocabulary learning: Input, instruction, involvement. In Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (pp. 343-354). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315716893-25 DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315716893-25

Laufer, B. (2020). Lexical coverages, inferencing unknown words and reading comprehension. TESOL Quarterly, 54(4), 1076-1085. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.3004 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.3004

Lin, C. J., & Mubarok, H. (2021). Learning analytics for investigating the mind map-guided AI chatbot approach in an EFL flipped speaking classroom. Educational Technology & Society, 24(4), 16-35.

Martínez-Fernández, A. (2008, September). Revisiting the involvement load hypothesis: Awareness, type of task and type of item. In Selected proceedings of the 2007 second language research forum (pp. 210-228). Cascadilla Press.

Nation, I. S. P. (2022). The Cambridge applied linguistics series. In Learning vocabulary in another language (pp. ii–iv). series-page, Cambridge University Press.

Nation, I. (2006). How large a vocabulary is needed for reading and listening?. Canadian Modern Language Review, 63(1), 59-82. https://doi.org/10.3138/cmlr.63.1.59 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3138/cmlr.63.1.59

Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1999). Syntactic priming in language production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(4), 136-141. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01293-0 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01293-0

Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Routinization in the interactive-alignment model of dialogue. Twenty-first century psycholinguistics: Four cornerstones.

Peng, J., Wang, C., & Lu, X. (2020). Effect of the linguistic complexity of the input text on alignment, writing fluency, and writing accuracy in the continuation task. Language Teaching Research, 24(3), 364-381. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168818783341 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168818783341

Rastle, K., Harrington, J., & Coltheart, M. (2002). 358,534 nonwords: The ARC nonword database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 55(4), 1339-1362.https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980244000099 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980244000099

Rieder, A. (2002). A cognitive view of incidental vocabulary acquisition: From text meaning to word meaning. Views, 11(1), 2.

Schmitt, N. (2008). Instructed second language vocabulary learning. Language Teaching Research, 12(3), 329-363. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168808089921 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168808089921

Shi, B., Huang, L., & Lu, X. (2020). Effect of prompt type on test-takers’ writing performance and writing strategy use in the continuation task. Language Testing, 37(3), 361-388. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265532220911626 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0265532220911626

Song, T., & Reynolds, B. L. (2022). The effects of lexical coverage and topic familiarity on the comprehension of L2 expository texts. TESOL Quarterly, 56(2). https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.3100 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.3100

Wang, C. M. (2015). Why does the continuation task facilitate L2 learning. Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 47(5), 753-762.

Ye, W., & Ren, W. (2019). Source use in the story continuation writing task. Assessing Writing, 39, 39-49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.asw.2018.12.001 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.asw.2018.12.001

Ye, W., Ren, W., & Zheng, S. (2021). The construct of the story continuation writing task: Insights from the China’s standards of English language ability. Chinese Journal of Applied Linguistics, 44(3), 382-398. https://doi.org/10.1515/CJAL-2021-0024 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/CJAL-2021-0024

Zhao, T., & Macaro, E. (2016). What works better for the learning of concrete and abstract words: Teachers’ L1 use or L2‐only explanations?. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 26(1), 75-98. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijal.12080 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/ijal.12080

Zhang, P., & Graham, S. (2020). Learning vocabulary through listening: The role of vocabulary knowledge and listening proficiency. Language Learning, 70(4), 1017-1053. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12411 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12411

Zhong, J. (2021). Investigating the influence of genre type on students' English writing performance in the continuation task (Master's thesis, Queen's University (Canada)).

Zhou, X., & Wang, C. (2024). Effects of interactive alignment on L2 vocabulary learning by Chinese EFL learners. Language Teaching Research, 28(2), 466-496. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688211004629 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688211004629

Ye (2024)

Downloads

Published

2024-08-08

Issue

Section

Original Research

How to Cite

Ye, Z. (2024). The effects of lexical densities and task types on Chinese students’ incidental vocabulary acquisition. Journal of Language Teaching, 4(3), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.54475/jlt.2024.014

Similar Articles

1-10 of 59

You may also start an advanced similarity search for this article.